banner

News

Jun 27, 2023

Light exposure behaviors predict mood, memory and sleep quality

Scientific Reports volume 13, Article number: 12425 (2023) Cite this article

683 Accesses

48 Altmetric

Metrics details

Ample research has shown that light influences our emotions, cognition, and sleep quality. However, little work has examined whether different light exposure-related behaviors, such as daytime exposure to electric light and nighttime usage of gadgets, especially before sleep, influence sleep quality and cognition. Three-hundred-and-one Malaysian adults (MeanAge±SD = 28 ± 9) completed the Light Exposure Behavior Assessment tool that measured five light exposure behaviors. They also completed the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, and single items assessing trouble in memory and concentration. A partial least square structural equation model, showing 72.72% predictive power, revealed that less use of wearable blue filters outdoors during the day and more within one hour before sleep predicted early peak time (direct effect = −0.25). Increased time spent outdoors predicted a positive affect (direct effect = 0.33) and a circadian phase advancement (direct effect: rising time = 0.14, peak time = 0.20, retiring time = 0.17). Increased use of mobile phone before sleep predicted a circadian phase delay (direct effect: retiring time = −0.25; rising time = −0.23; peak time = −0.22; morning affect = −0.12), reduced sleep quality (direct effect = 0.13), and increased trouble in memory and concentration (total effect = 0.20 and 0.23, respectively). Increased use of tunable, LED, or dawn-simulating electric light in the morning and daytime predicted a circadian phase advancement (direct effect: peak time = 0.15, morning affect = 0.14, retiring time = 0.15) and good sleep quality (direct effect = −0.16). The results provide valuable insights into developing a healthy light diet to promote health and wellness.

Scientific evidence published over the last four decades has shown that retinal light exposure influences our physiology, behavior, and emotion. More specifically, it modulates human sleep, circadian rhythms, alertness, mood, neuroendocrine and neurobehavioral functions1,2,3,4,5. These influences of light on human physiology and behaviors are collectively known as non-image-forming responses (NIF) of light. The melanopsin-enriched intrinsically photoreceptive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs), sensitive to short wavelength-enriched (blue-enriched, ~ 480 nm) light6, generally mediate the NIF effects of light.

With the advent of artificial light and self-luminous displays, our retinal light exposure is no more limited to the natural day-night cycle. An extensive body of research suggests that the imbalance of light and dark exposure disrupts our circadian system7. Subsequently, this disruption gives rise to a series of adverse consequences, including decreased sleep quality, mood, and an alteration of sleeping habits7,8,9. Since the natural light–dark cycle is the most vital zeitgeber to synchronize our body clock to the astronomical day, altering this cycle forces us to have different chronotype-disposition for activity early or late in the day10. Research shows that exposure to bright light (~ 5000–10,000 lux) at night results in a phase delay11, and exposure to bright light in the morning leads to a phase advance12,13. Increased nighttime light exposure is also associated with decreased sleep quality14,15. However, several studies have reported better nighttime sleep quality after exposure to electric light (300–1000 lux) in the morning9,16,17,18. He et al.17 observed a higher nocturnal sleep efficiency, earlier sleep onset, shorter sleep latency, and lower morning sleepiness among college students (N = 12) when they are exposed to bright light (1000 lux, 6500 K) in the morning for five days compared to conventional office light (300 lux, 4000 K). Brain regions such as limbic areas and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis responsible for regulating mood are susceptible to circadian regulation19. Thus, it is reasonable to anticipate that the disruption of circadian regulation will disrupt mood regulation19. Bright light exposure in the morning increases positive mood; however, exposure to bright light in the afternoon enhances negative mood20,21,22,23.

Several studies have confirmed that retinal light exposure activates the hippocampus, which is closely associated with memory functions24,25,26. Thus, researchers anticipate that retinal light exposure would influence memory. Vandewalle et al.27 observed that, compared to 18 min of exposure to a monochromatic green light (550 nm; 3 × 1013 photons/cm2/s), 18 min of exposure to a monochromatic blue light (470 nm; 3 × 1013 photons/cm2/s) improves working memory performance (N = 18). Alkozei et al.28 reported enhanced verbal memory for a 30-min monochromatic blue light exposure (469 nm; 214 lux; N = 12) compared to monochromatic amber light (578 nm; 188 lux). Huiberts et al.29 offered further evidence that light influences memory-based task performances, whereby participants performed better in simple than complex tasks under polychromatic white bright light conditions (200 lux, 4000 K vs. 1000 lux, 4000 K; N = 64). Retinal light exposure is also associated with improved concentration. Kretschmer et al.30 observed an improved concentration among night shift workers (N = 32) under a bright light condition (3269–3684 lux vs. 257–339 lux). Sleegers et al.31, in their studies on the effects of light in classroom environments, concluded a beneficial influence of a dynamic light environment on students’ concentration (1000 lux, 6500 K vs. 300 lux, 3000–4000 K; N = 181).

Due to social jetlag (misaligned sleep–wake pattern with work schedule), different chronotypes, especially early and late chronotypes, might exhibit a reduced sleep quality. Juda et al.32 found that workers with early chronotypes had shorter sleep duration and more sleep disturbances than late chronotypes (N = 371 shift workers). Moreover, late chronotypes had poor sleep quality with non-regular sleeping habits during weekdays due to the misalignment of their preferred activity period vs. real-world demands33,34,35. Chronotypes can also influence our memory and concentration36,37,38. Schmidt et al.36 reported an interaction effect of chronotype and time of day on memory (N = 32). The memory performance of those with early chronotype was better in the morning. In the same vein, the memory performance of those with late chronotype was better in the afternoon39,40. Researchers have termed it the synchrony effect. Research has also indicated that sleep quality is contingent on mood and vice versa41,42. Positive affect- a state of pleasurable engagement with the environment, is associated with improved sleep patterns43,44. In contrast, negative affect (feelings of emotional distress) leads to sleep deprivation, poor sleep quality, and reduced cognitive functioning45,46,47,48,49. Poor sleep quality, the core symptom of mood disorder, is associated with decreased positive affect42. Poor sleep quality also reduces memory functions and concentration50,51,52,53,54.

Acknowledging the influence of retinal light exposure on our health and well-being, many researchers have attempted to quantify healthy light exposure. They have given recommendations for a healthy indoor light environment that primarily focuses on properties of the light spectrum, such as illuminance and wavelength55. However, little effort is visible to study light exposure-related behaviors, which could be an active agent modifying our retinal light exposure. People can control their light exposure through different behaviors by actively seeking or avoiding certain types of light exposure. There is a knowledge gap in understanding these behaviors, which is crucial to developing a healthy light diet- a light exposure pattern promoting health, wellness, and work performance. To address this gap, we have developed the Light Exposure Behavior Assessment (LEBA)56 tool, which will facilitate understanding people’s light exposure related behaviors and the development of a healthy light diet. LEBA categorizes five different types of behavior. First, the propensity of wearing blue light filter glasses indoors and outdoors (LEBA B1). Second, the tendency to spend time outdoors (LEBA B2). Third, the usage of mobile phones on the bed before sleeping (LEBA B3). Fourth, our inclination to control environmental light before bedtime (LEBA B4). Finally, the use of electric light (LEBA B5). However, whether these categorizations of behaviors would predict different aspects of our health, memory and concentration remain unknown.

We posed the following questions: What are the influences of LEBA categories on (a) chronotype, (b) mood, (c) sleep quality, and (d) memory and concentration? To answer these questions, we proposed a theoretical framework (Fig. 1) based on the literature reviewed. We used the partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM)—most suited to formulate such a predictive model57,58. Predicting relationships using PLS-SEM is a two-step process. First, a measurement model is used to assess the reliability and validity of the latent variables used in the model. Second, a structural model is used to investigate the predicted relationships of the latent structures. In the structural model, (i) the direct effects (DE): influences unmediated by any other constructs in the model, (ii) indirect effects (IE): influences mediated by at least one intervening construct in the model and (iii) total effects (TE): sums of direct and indirect effects of a given construct can be estimated59.

Theoretical framework of the fitted PLS-SEM model to predict chronotype, sleep quality, mood, memory and concentration using LEBA categories as predictors.

We predicted that five types of LEBA behavior categories would directly influence chronotype (H1), mood (H2), and sleep quality (H3). We also predicted a bidirectional relationship between mood and sleep quality (H4). Additionally, we predicted that chronotype (H5) would influence sleep quality. Sleep quality (H6), mood (H7), and chronotype (H8) would affect memory and concentration. LEBA categories would directly influence memory and concentration (H9). Lastly, we predicted that LEBA categories would exhibit a significant total effect on sleep quality (H10), memory, and concentration (H11).

We conducted a large-scale online survey on Malaysian residents. The exclusion-inclusion criteria for respondents to be included in this study were: (1) any Malaysian resident aged > 18 and able to read and write English (2) no physiological and psychological disorder (self-reported). Three hundred and sixty-six adults completed the survey. The completion rate of our survey was 87% (45 participants' data was excluded due to incompleteness). We further excluded 19 participants based on our exclusion-inclusion criteria. Thus, we used data from 301 participants for further processing.

A priori power analysis was conducted to determine the sample size adequacy with G*Power 3.060. To achieve an effect size of 0.1561 and 80% statistical power and \(\alpha\) = 0.05, for a multiple liner regression with 13 predictors, a total sample size of 131 individuals was needed. Further, the maximum number of items per factor in our model was six. In the PLS-SEM-based analysis, to detect a minimum \({R}^{2}\) value of 0.10 for a factor with six items with 80% statistical power and α = 0.05, at least 130 participants are required62. Our sample size exceeded these recommendations.

Light exposure-related behaviors were measured using the short form of the Light Exposure Behavior Assessment56. The short form contains five factors with 18 items. Light Exposure Behavior Assessment (LEBA) measures the propensity of different light exposure-related behaviors in the last month retrospectively using a five-point Likert-type response scale (1 = never; 2 = rarely; 3 = sometimes; 4 = often; 5 = always).

The positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS)63 was used to measure positive (PA) and negative affect (NA) with two 10-item mood scales. Participants retrospectively rated their positive and negative affect based on the last month using a five-point Likert-type response scale (1 = very slightly/not at all; 2 = a little; 3 = moderately; 4 = quite a bit; 5 = extremely).

We developed two single global items with four-point Likert-type response options investigating trouble in memory and concentration. These single global items asked the participants about the propensity of their memory and concentration difficulty in the last month (0 = Absent; 1 = Slight; 2 = Moderate; 3 = Severe).

We used the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI)64 to measure the participants' sleep quality. PSQI measures seven domains of sleep to differentiate “poor” from “good” sleep. Participants responded to the PSQI using Likert-type response options ranging from 0 to 3, whereby 3 reflects the negative extreme on the Likert Scale. A sum of scores ≥ 5 indicates poor sleep quality. The latent structure of PSQI was reported to vary from one factor to three factors64,65. Dunleavy et al.66, in their study, recommended using a two-factor model: perceived sleep quality (PSQ) and sleep efficiency (SE) while measuring the sleep quality among Singapore citizens. In this study, we followed their recommended structure. A higher score in the PSQ factor would indicate decreased perceived sleep quality. In contrast, a higher score in the SE factor would indicate higher sleep efficiency.

Chronotype was measured using Morningness-Eveningness questionnaire (MEQ)67. MEQ consists of 19 questions, and the scores range from 16 to 86. A higher score indicates a higher morning propensity. Caci et al.68 reported a four-factor structure of MEQ: peak time (PT), morning affect (MA), retiring time (RT) and rising time (RI) in a student sample (N = 456). Items in PT assess the body’s peak time for different activities. MA assesses our bodily responses in the morning. RT assesses the time when our body starts to prepare for sleeping. Lastly, RI investigates the time when our body prepares for waking up.

The project was approved by Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee (Project ID: 14,786). The research was performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines/regulations of the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained from all participants before data collection. This was a cross-sectional, fully anonymous online survey. Participants were invited via email and social media (i.e., LinkedIn, Twitter, and Facebook) with the attachment of an Explanatory Statement in which we mentioned that their participation would be voluntary and that they could withdraw from participation at any time without being penalized. If the participants expressed happiness with the statement, a survey link was sent to them. At the beginning of the survey, their consent was recorded digitally. The survey took 15–20 min, for which they were not compensated. The survey was completed between April and November, 2022.

We used R (4.1.2v)69 and several statistical packages, including esemComp70, “SEMinR71” and tabledown72 for our analysis.

We gathered structural validity evidence of LEBA, PSQI, MEQ and PANAS scales in our sample using the exploratory structural equation modeling (ESEM)73. ESEM intricates the computational advantages of exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis by allowing the items to cross-load to represent the data more realistically and offering fit indices to assess the model fit. To assess the model fit, we followed the guidelines of Hu and Bentler74: comparative fit index (CFI) and the Tucker Lewis index (TLI): acceptable fit \(\ge 0\).90, good fit \(\ge 0\).95; the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA): acceptable fit < 0.08, good fit < 0.06; and the standardized root mean square (SRMR): acceptable fit < 0.10, good fit < 0.08.

First, we assessed the quality of the measurement model. We excluded items with factor loading < 0.40 to increase the robustness of the measurement model71. Second, we estimated the internal consistency reliability estimates of each construct. We reported both the lower bound estimate of reliability- Cronbach’s \(\alpha\) coefficient and the upper bound estimate of reliability-construct reliability (CR). Both Cronbach’s \(\alpha\) and CR coefficient values range between 0 and 1, where higher values represent better reliability. As a general guideline, Cronbach’s \(\alpha\) above 0.70 is considered satisfactory75,76 and a value above 0.50 is considered acceptable77. CR coefficient value of 0.60 and above indicates a satisfactory reliability71.

Third, we assessed the convergent and discriminant validity of the measurement model. For convergent validity, we used each construct's average variance extracted (AVE) value. AVE ≥ 0.50 or AVE < 0.50 with a CR > 0.60 and AVE < CR indicate an acceptable convergent validity78. For discriminant validity, we compared the square root of the AVE of a construct with its corresponding correlation with other constructs78. The square root of the AVEs of each construct should be higher than its correlation with other constructs. We have also reported the bootstrapped heterotrait-monotrait ratio (HTMT) of correlations of the construct as additional proof of discriminant validity. For conceptually similar constructs, the HTMT value should be < 0.90; for constructs that are conceptually distinct, the HTMT value should be < 0.8079.

First, we assessed the collinearity of the constructs in our structural model by calculating variance inflation factor (VIF) values. VIF > 3 indicates probable collinearity issues79. Next, we estimated the direct effects (DE) and total effects (TE) of the structural model using a bootstrapping approach with 10,000 sub-samples and reported the significant total effects (t > 1.96) observed in our model. Lastly, we reported the adjusted \({R}^{2}\) as a measure of the explanatory power. For assessing the explanatory power, we followed the guidelines of Falk and Miller80: \({R}^{2}\) values \(\ge\) 0.10 indicates adequate explanatory power. Further, we have categorized the \({R}^{2}\) values following the guidelines of Cohen61: 0.02 (weak), 0.13 (moderate), and 0.26 (substantial). For predictive relevance, we assessed the fitted model’s predictive power by K-fold cross-validation using the \(PL{S}_{predict}\) function from the “SEMinR” package71. \(PL{S}_{predict}\) provides the root-mean-square error (RMSE) and respective linear-regression model benchmarks (LM) for all indicators. We assessed the model’s predictive power by following the guideline of Hair71: (i) high predictive power: all indicators in the fitted PLS-SEM model have lower RMSE values compared to the LM (ii) medium predictive power: the majority(≥ 50%) of the indicators have lower RMSE values than LM (iii) low predictive power: less than 50% of the indicator have lower RMSE value than LM (iv) no predictive power: no indicator has lower RMSE value than LM model. Figure 2 depicts the analysis steps we followed.

Analyses steps used in the study.

The project received ethics clearance from Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee (Project ID: 14,786). Informed consent was obtained from all participants. Participants were assured about their voluntary participation and the right to withdraw at any time.

Out of 301 participants (AgeMean±SD = 28 ± 9), 218 (72.43%) were female, ranging in age from 18 to 59 years (AgeMean±SD = 26.85 ± 8.07), and 83 (27.57%) were male with an age range between 18 to 74 years (AgeMean±SD = 30.35 ± 12.14): 78.66% were unmarried. The majority of the participants were students (71.42%) and of intermediate chronotype (68%). Table 1 summarizes the participants' demographics and descriptive statistics of the measures. Figure 3. depicts the response distribution, mean score and SD for each item of LEBA.

Response distribution of Light Exposure Behavior Assessment.

Table 2 presents the fit indices of the scales used in this study. Light Exposure Behavior Assessment (LEBA), Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ), and Positive and Negative Affect Scales (PANAS) exhibited acceptable to a good fit in terms of CFI and TLI (> 0.95 or 0.90), RMSEA (< 0.08 or 0.06), and SRMR (< 0.08).

We excluded two items from LEBA (items 02 & 04) and four items from MEQ (items 06, 10, 16, 12) due to weak factor loadings (< 0.40; Supplementary Table S1). The results of the refitted measurement model assessment are presented in Supplementary Table S2. The sleep efficiency (SE) factor of PSQI exhibited poor reliability in terms of Cronbach’s \(\alpha\) (\(\alpha\)= 0.48) but had satisfactory construct reliability (CR = 0.79). All other factors exhibited acceptable to satisfactory internal consistency in terms of Cronbach’s \(\alpha\) (0.51–0.94) and construct reliability (0.72–0.96). Concerning convergent validity, 8 out of 13 constructs had average variance extracted (AVE) > 0.50 (except LEBA B2, negative affect, perceived sleep quality, peak time and retiring). However, all 13 constructs had CR > 0.60 and AVE < CR. This indicated acceptable reliability and convergent validity of all constructs in the model.

To establish the discriminant validity, we calculated the square root of each construct’s AVE and compared them to their corresponding inter-construct correlation (Supplementary Table S3). All constructs’ square root of AVE were greater than their inter-construct correlation indicating satisfactory discriminant validity. Further evidence of the discriminant validity of the constructs was drawn by heterotrait-monotrait (HTMT) analysis. Supplementary Table S4 presents the HTMT values and indicates satisfactory discriminant validity (HTMT < 0.80) for all 13 constructs.

Variance inflation factors (VIF) for all constructs were < 3, indicating no possible collinearity. Table 3 depict significant (t > 1.96) direct effects and total effects observed in our model. All direct effects of the structural model are provided in Supplementary Table S5.

Table 3 shows that, in line with our predictions, LEBA categories exhibited direct effects on chronotype (H1), mood (H2) and sleep quality (H3). Among the four factors of chronotype, we observed a negative direct effect of wearing blue light filter glasses (LEBA B1) on peak time (PT; body’s peak time for different activities). Less use of blue light filter glasses indoors during the day and more within one hour before sleep (LEBA B1) predicted early peak time (PT; \(\beta\)= −0.25). Spending time outdoors (LEBA B2) exhibited a direct effect on positive affect (\(\beta\)= 0.33) and chronotype factors: peak time (PT; \(\beta\)= 0.20), retiring time (RT; the time when our body starts to prepare for sleeping; \(\beta\)= 0.17), and rising time (RI; the time when our body prepares for waking up; \(\beta\)= 0.14). In contrast, the usage of mobile phones on the bed before sleeping (LEBA B3) directly but negatively, influenced the four chronotype factors: PT (\(\beta\)= −0.22), morning affect (MA; \(\beta\)= −0.12), RT (\(\beta\)= −0.25) and RI (\(\beta\)= −0.23) and predicted decreased perceived sleep quality (PSQ; \(\beta\)= 0.13; a higher score indicated poor sleep quality). The increased use of electric light during daytime (LEBA B5) positively influenced chronotype factors: PT (\(\beta\)= 0.15), MA (\(\beta\)= 0.14) and RT (\(\beta\)= 0.15) and increased perceived sleep quality (PSQ; \(\beta\)= −0.16; a lower score indicated higher sleep quality). But this behavior dimension (LEBA B5) was associated with increased negative affect (\(\beta\)= 0.19). Results indicated a bidirectional relationship between mood and sleep quality. Positive and negative affect directly influenced sleep quality, and vise-versa (H4). Positive mood increased both the sleep efficiency (SE; \(\beta\)= 0.21) and PSQ (\(\beta\)= −0.19), whereas negative affect decreased PSQ ( \(\beta\)= 0.28). Again, better SE and PSQ predicted better PA (\(\beta\)= 0.21 and −0.29). Better PSQ was predicted less NA (\(\beta\)= 0.37).

Chronotype directly influenced sleep quality (H5), where increased MA was observed to increase PSQ (\(\beta\)= −0.21). A negative influence of PSQ was observed on memory and concentration (H6), whereby poor PSQ was predicted to increase trouble in memory (\(\beta\)= 0.13) and concentration (\(\beta\)= 0.22). Increased negative affect predicted a deteriorated memory and concentration (H7; memory = 0.37; concentration = 0.32). We also observed direct effect of chronotype on trouble in memory and concentration (H8). Increased morning affect was predicted to decrease trouble in memory and concentration (\(\beta\)= −0.13 and −0.11). However, no significant direct effect of light exposure-related behaviors (H9) was observed on trouble in memory and concentration.

We observed significant total effects of light exposure-related behaviors on sleep quality (H10). The usage of mobile phones on the bed before sleeping (LEBA B3) predicted the decrease of PSQ (\(\beta\)= 0.20), whereas increased use of electric light during daytime (LEBA B5) increased PSQ (\(\beta\)= −0.18). Lastly, significant total effects of light exposure-related behaviors on memory and concentration were observed (H11). The usage of mobile phones on the bed before sleeping (LEBA B3) predicted an increase of memory and concentration problem (\(\beta\)= 0.20 and \(\beta\)= 0.23, respectively).

Our fitted model exhibited substantial explanatory power (R2) for PSQ (26.70%), trouble in concentration (31.67%) and trouble in memory (27.32%). Moderate R2 was observed for PA (25.27%), NA (18.03%), PT (14.58%) and RT (13.32%). Our model exhibited weak R2 for MA (4%), RI (9%), and SE (4%). \(PL{S}_{predict}\) function indicated our model had medium predictive power with 72.72% of the indicators having RMSE value lower than the LM benchmark.

This study investigated if light exposure behaviors predict chronotype, sleep quality, mood, memory, and concentration. Preliminary analyses revealed that light-exposure behaviors affected sleep quality, concentration, and memory. Results from LEBA indicated that participants generally used blue light filters less often, spent less time outdoors, and were highly engaged in mobile phones in bed before sleep. These behaviors could have contributed to poor sleep quality and trouble in memory and concentration. The results strengthened the need for a model to predict how light exposure behaviors explain human cognition and sleep quality.

The measurement models indicated acceptable reliability and validity of the scales we used to measure chronotype, sleep quality, and mood. Two factors: sleep efficiency (SE) and rising time (RI), had Cronbach’s α < 0.60 but exhibited satisfactory construct reliability (> 0.60). These two factors were composed of only two items each, which might have contributed to the low Cronbach’s alpha coefficient. Further, we only asked two separate questions to assess if the participants experienced any trouble with recalling memory and concentration. Using such global single items allowed us to reduce participants’ cognitive demands required to respond to the survey and increased the response rate81. Typically, single questions are found reliable with good predictive validity and allow the participants to consider the key features of the given construct82,83,84,85.

Results indicated that the structural model had satisfactory explanatory power (R2 > 0.10) for all factors except for morning affect (MA), rising time (RI), and sleep efficiency (SE). These three factors exhibited weak R2. One possible reason could be that they are influenced by other factors not included in the model, such as genetics, time of day, and work schedule. In any case, our models generally exhibited satisfactory predictive relevance, and most relationships confirmed our predictions.

Wearing blue light filters (LEBA B1) influenced peak time directly—a chronotype indicating the body’s peak time for different activities. Lower usage of blue light filters indoors during the day and higher usage at night, especially one hour before sleep, predicted a circadian phase advancement, meaning our body starts functioning earlier than the usual time (Direct effect, DE = −0.21). The results support previous studies that showed the blue light exposure during daytime and the absence of blue light at night was responsible for synchronizing our body clock with the natural light–dark cycle and preparing our body to rise early86,87. A group of photoreceptors in our eye—intrinsically photoreceptive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs) are sensitive to blue light2,6. These ipRGCs receive signals from the light and send them to the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) region of our brain, the so-called master clock of our body clock, to align our inner rhythm with the astronomical cycle. Hence, deprivation of blue light during the daytime, especially in the morning, and exposure at night misguides our circadian rhythm. Figueiro et al.88 reported that blue-enriched light exposure throughout the day promotes better alignment of the circadian rhythm with the earth’s 24-h light–dark cycle. Figueiro and Rea89 observed a delay in nighttime melatonin onset due to blue-depleted daytime light exposure (from the awakening time until approximately 15:00), causing a circadian phase delay.

Spending time outdoors (LEBA B2) predicted an improved mood in our participants by increasing their positive affect. Previous studies also reported similar results8,90. An et al.90 observed reduced depressive mood in workers when more sunlight was available in their workplace. Figueiro et al.8 found fewer depressive symptoms for light exposures with high circadian efficiency—an ability to entrain our body clock like the sunlight. We observed a positive direct effect of spending time outdoors on chronotype. It indicates a potential relationship between exposure to outdoor light and phase advancement in our circadian rhythm (see also8,91). After analyzing a bio-bank of 400,000 UK participants, Burns et al.91 reported that time spent in outdoor light promoted phase advancement. Also, there could be a possibility that people who have early chronotypes might have the advantage of spending more time outdoors than those with late chronotypes. The results suggest that sleep and mood-related problems could be rooted in people’s light exposure-related behaviors.

Increased use of smart gadgets (mobile phones) in bed before night sleep (LEBA B3) predicted the phase delay and reduced sleep quality. This exposure-related behavior also harmed memory performance and concentration. Past research revealed adverse effects of using smart devices in bed on sleep quality92,93,94. The self-luminous display of smart gadgets often emits blue lights, exposure to which at night is directly associated with reduced cognitive functioning, circadian phase shift, and reduced sleep quality95,96,97,98,99.

Results indicated that the increased use of electric light (tunable, LED, or dawn-simulating light) in the morning and daytime (LEBA B5) increased sleep quality and predicated a circadian phase advancement. Figueiro et al.8 found similar results, whereby increased circadian daytime light exposure improved sleep quality among office workers. Several studies independently demonstrated that inadequate daytime light exposure caused increased melatonin suppression at night, resulting in a circadian phase delay, more nighttime awakening, sleep deprivation, and poor sleep quality100,101,102. Studies based on real-world settings such as offices and schools also indicated that increased electric light exposure improved sleep quality9,103,104. However, increased use of these electric lights in the morning also predicted increased negative affect. The use of electric lights in the morning and during the day could be associated with the inaccessibility of sunlight, which might contribute to increased negative affect105.

Unexpectedly, we did not observe any influence of the factor—Controlling the light environment before bedtime (LEBA B4) on sleep, emotion, and cognition. The participants reported if they controlled the light emitted from their devices before bedtime, such as if they used blue light filter applications or dimmed the monitor one hour before sleep. But, recent recommendations indicated that investigations related to light in a sleep environment should consider a time span of three hours prior to sleep55, which could be a contributing factor to such findings. Additionally, the effects of light before bedtime could also depend on light exposure history and characteristics of surrounding light106, which were not accounted for in our model.

Results indicated that increased morning affect (H8), a factor of chronotype, predicted less trouble in memory and concentration. Although the circadian phase advancement enhanced memory and concentration, the relationship seemed more complex because other factors might influence this relationship. For example, early chronotypes may be less susceptible to social jetlag (misaligned sleep–wake pattern with work schedule); hence, people might subsequently experience fewer issues with memory and concentration than other chronotypes107,108.

We like to mention several limitations of this study. First, we fitted the PLS-SEM-based model on a female-dominated sample that hinders the generalizability of the results. Future studies should recruit a gender-balanced sample with higher representativeness of the multi-ethnic Malaysian population and fit the model to balanced subgroups such as ethnicity or age. Second, morning affect (MA), rising behaviors (RI), and sleep efficiency (SE) exhibited weak R2 in our fitted model. Further research with larger samples and more comprehensive measures for additional variables might be necessary to improve the explanatory power of the fitted model. Third, the importance of considering the time of the day when accounting for the effects of light exposure cannot be overstated. However, we should note that most of the behavioral dimensions of LEBA do not objectively address the time of the day. To improve the accuracy and reliability of future studies, we recommend that researchers consider the role of time of the day as a variable and develop a model that incorporates it. Fourth, there was an underrepresentation of elderly participants (> 65 years of age) in this study. Participants' age is a critical factor that can significantly influence light exposure-related behavior, which raises concerns about the generalizability of the study's findings to the older population.

This research investigated whether light exposure-related behaviors could predict chronotype, sleep quality, mood, memory, and concentration. Our goal was to devise a healthy light diet. We first developed a conceptual framework and then applied a partial least square structural equation modeling to data gathered from 301 Malaysian adults. All the constructs used in the model exhibited acceptable reliability and validity. Results indicated that the less usage of wearable blue filters outdoors during the day and more within one hour before sleep predicted a circadian phase advancement. Also, spending time outdoors promotes mood and circadian phase advancement. However, using gadgets (mobile phones) in bed before sleeping negatively affected mood, sleep quality, memory, and concentration. The former also predicted a circadian phase delay. Using electric light (tunable, LED, or dawn-simulating light) in the morning and during the daytime promotes circadian phase advancement and enhances sleep quality. Generally, these findings would help develop a healthy light diet to facilitate health and wellness.

The datasets generated and/analyzed during the current study are available in the GitHub repository, https://github.com/ILLMMU/Study2.

Lok, R., Smolders, K. C., Beersma, D. G., Domien, J. G. & de Kort, Y. A. Light, alertness, and alerting effects of white light: A literature overview. J. Biol. Rhythms 33, 589–601. https://doi.org/10.1177/0748730418796443 (2018).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Lockley, S. W. Spectral sensitivity of circadian, neuroendocrine and neurobehavioral effects of light. J. Human–Environ. Syst. 11, 43 (2008).

Article Google Scholar

Cajochen, C. Alerting effects of light. Sleep Med. Rev. 11, 453–464. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2007.07.009 (2007).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Siraji, M., Kalavally, V., Schaefer, A. & Haque, S. Effects of daytime electric light exposure on human alertness and higher cognitive functions: A systematic review. Front. Psychol. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.765750 (2022).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Xiao, H., Cai, H. & Li, X. Non-visual effects of indoor light environment on humans: A review. Physiol. Behav. 228, 113195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113195 (2021).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Hankins, M. W. & Lucas, R. J. The primary visual pathway in humans is regulated according to long-term light exposure through the action of a nonclassical photopigment. Curr. Biol. 12, 191–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0960-9822(02)00659-0 (2002).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Lunn, R. M. et al. Health consequences of electric lighting practices in the modern world: A report on the National Toxicology Program’s workshop on shift work at night, artificial light at night, and circadian disruption. Sci. Total Environ. 607, 1073–1084 (2017).

Article ADS PubMed Google Scholar

Figueiro, M. G. et al. The impact of daytime light exposures on sleep and mood in office workers. Sleep Health 3, 204–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2017.03.005 (2017).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Viola, A. U., James, L. M., Schlangen, L. J. M. & Dijk, D.-J. Blue-enriched White Light in the Workplace Improves self-reported Alertness, performance and sleep quality. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 34, 297–306. https://doi.org/10.5271/sjweh.1268 (2008).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Porcheret, K. et al. Chronotype and environmental light exposure in a student population. Chronobiol. Int. 35, 1365–1374. https://doi.org/10.1080/07420528.2018.1482556 (2018).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Koo, Y. S. et al. Outdoor artificial light at night, obesity, and sleep health: Cross-sectional analysis in the KoGES study. Chronobiol. Int. 33, 301–314. https://doi.org/10.3109/07420528.2016.1143480 (2016).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Czeisler, C. A. et al. Bright light induction of strong (Type 0) resetting of the human circadian pacemaker. Science 244, 1328–1333. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2734611 (1989).

Article ADS CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Khalsa, S. B. S., Jewett, M. E., Cajochen, C. & Czeisler, C. A. A phase response curve to single bright light pulses in human subjects. J. Physiol. 549, 945–952. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2003.040477 (2003).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Cho, J. R., Joo, E. Y., Koo, D. L. & Hong, S. B. Let there be no light: The effect of bedside light on sleep quality and background electroencephalographic rhythms. Sleep Med. 14, 1422–1425. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2013.09.007 (2013).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Obayashi, K., Saeki, K. & Kurumatani, N. Association between light exposure at night and insomnia in the general elderly population: The HEIJO-KYO cohort. Chronobiol. Int. 31, 976–982. https://doi.org/10.3109/07420528.2014.937491 (2014).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Boubekri, M., Cheung, I. N., Reid, K. J., Wang, C.-H. & Zee, P. C. Impact of windows and daylight exposure on overall health and sleep quality of office workers: A case-control pilot study. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 10, 603–611. https://doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.3780 (2014).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

He, M., Ru, T., Li, S., Li, Y. & Zhou, G. Shine light on sleep: Morning bright light improves nocturnal sleep and next morning alertness among college students. J. Sleep Res. 32, e13724. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.13724 (2023).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Liu, C.-R., Liou, Y. M. & Jou, J.-H. Ambient bright lighting in the morning improves sleep disturbances of older adults with dementia. Sleep Med. 89, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2021.10.011 (2022).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Bedrosian, T. A. & Nelson, R. J. Timing of light exposure affects mood and brain circuits. Transl. Psychiatry 7, e1017–e1017. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2016.262 (2017).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Leichtfried, V. et al. Intense illumination in the morning hours improved mood and alertness but not mental performance. Appl. Ergon. 46, 54–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2014.07.001 (2015).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Borisuit, A., Linhart, F., Scartezzini, J. L. & Münch, M. Effects of realistic office daylighting and electric lighting conditions on visual comfort, alertness and mood. Lighting Res. Technol. (London, England: 2001) 47, 192–209. https://doi.org/10.1177/1477153514531518 (2015).

Article Google Scholar

Hoffmann, G. et al. Effects of variable lighting intensities and colour temperatures on sulphatoxymelatonin and subjective mood in an experimental office workplace. Appl. Ergon. 39, 719–728. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.apergo.2007.11.005 (2008).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Ru, T., de Kort, Y. A. W., Smolders, K. C. H. J., Chen, Q. & Zhou, G. Non-image forming effects of illuminance and correlated color temperature of office light on alertness, mood, and performance across cognitive domains. Build. Environ. 149, 253–263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.buildenv.2018.12.002 (2019).

Article Google Scholar

Vandewalle, G., Maquet, P. & Dijk, D.-J. Light as a modulator of cognitive brain function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 429–438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.07.004 (2009).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Vandewalle, G. et al. Spectral quality of light modulates emotional brain responses in humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 107, 19549–19554. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1010180107 (2010).

Article ADS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Hattar, S. et al. Central projections of melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion cells in the mouse. J. Comp. Neurol 497, 326–349. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.20970 (2006).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Vandewalle, G. et al. Wavelength-dependent modulation of brain responses to a working memory task by daytime light exposure. Cereb. Cortex 17, 2788–2795. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhm007 (2007).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Alkozei, A., Smith, R., Dailey, N. S., Bajaj, S. & Killgore, W. D. S. Acute exposure to blue wavelength light during memory consolidation improves verbal memory performance. PLoS ONE 12, e0184884. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0184884 (2017).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Huiberts, L. M., Smolders, K. C. H. J. & de Kort, Y. A. W. Shining light on memory: Effects of bright light on working memory performance. Behav. Brain Res. 294, 234–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.07.045 (2015).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Kretschmer, V., Schmidt, K. H. & Griefahn, B. Bright light effects on working memory, sustained attention and concentration of elderly night shift workers. Lighting Res. Technol. 44, 316–333. https://doi.org/10.1177/1477153511418769 (2012).

Article Google Scholar

Sleegers, P. et al. Lighting affects students’ concentration positively: Findings from three Dutch studies. Light. Res. Technol. 45, 159–175. https://doi.org/10.1177/1477153512446099 (2013).

Article Google Scholar

Juda, M., Vetter, C. & Roenneberg, T. Chronotype modulates sleep duration, sleep quality, and social jet lag in shift-workers. J. Biol. Rhythms 28, 141–151. https://doi.org/10.1177/0748730412475042 (2013).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Vitale, J. A. et al. Chronotype influences activity circadian rhythm and sleep: Differences in sleep quality between weekdays and weekend. Chronobiol. Int. 32, 405–415. https://doi.org/10.3109/07420528.2014.986273 (2015).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Taillard, J., Philip, P. & Bioulac, B. Morningness/eveningness and the need for sleep. J. Sleep Res. 8, 291–295. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2869.1999.00176.x (1999).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Sukegawa, M. et al. Sleep and lifestyle habits in morning and evening types of human circadian rhythm. Biol. Rhythm. Res. 40, 121–127. https://doi.org/10.1080/09291010701794404 (2009).

Article Google Scholar

Schmidt, C. et al. Pushing the limits: Chronotype and time of day modulate working memory-dependent cerebral activity. Front. Neurol. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2015.00199 (2015).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Rosenthal, L. et al. Sleepiness/alertness among healthy evening and morning type individuals. Sleep Med. 2, 243–248. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1389-9457(00)00047-2 (2001).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Matchock, R. L. & Toby Mordkoff, J. Chronotype and time-of-day influences on the alerting, orienting, and executive components of attention. Exp. Brain Res. 192, 189–198. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1567-6 (2009).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

May, C. P. & Hasher, L. Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. J. Exper. Psychol. Human Percept. Perform. 24, 363–379. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.24.2.363 (1998).

Article CAS Google Scholar

Hidalgo, M. P. L. et al. Performance of chronotypes on memory tests during the morning and the evening shifts. Psychol. Rep. 95, 75–85. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.95.1.75-85 (2004).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Ong, A. D., Kim, S., Young, S. & Steptoe, A. Positive affect and sleep: A systematic review. Sleep Med. Rev. 35, 21–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2016.07.006 (2017).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Bower, B., Bylsma, L. M., Morris, B. H. & Rottenberg, J. Poor reported sleep quality predicts low positive affect in daily life among healthy and mood-disordered persons. J. Sleep Res. 19, 323–332. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2009.00816.x (2010).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Steptoe, A., O’Donnell, K., Marmot, M. & Wardle, J. Positive affect, psychological well-being, and good sleep. J. Psychosom. Res. 64, 409–415. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychores.2007.11.008 (2008).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Fosse, R., Stickgold, R. & Hobson, J. A. Emotional experience during rapid-eye-movement sleep in narcolepsy. Sleep 25, 724–732. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/25.7.724 (2002).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Johnson, E. O., Roth, T. & Breslau, N. The association of insomnia with anxiety disorders and depression: Exploration of the direction of risk. J. Psychiatr. Res. 40, 700–708. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.07.008 (2006).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Riemann, D. et al. The hyperarousal model of insomnia: A review of the concept and its evidence. Sleep Med. Rev. 14, 19–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2009.04.002 (2009).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Perlstein, W. M., Elbert, T. & Stenger, V. A. Dissociation in human prefrontal cortex of affective influences on working memory-related activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 99, 1736–1741. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.241650598 (2002).

Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Sharifian, N. & Zahodne, L. B. Daily associations between social media use and memory failures: the mediating role of negative affect. J. Gen. Psychol. 148, 67–83. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221309.2020.1743228 (2021).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Threadgill, A. H. & Gable, P. A. Negative affect varying in motivational intensity influences scope of memory. Cogn. Emot. 33, 332–345. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2018.1451306 (2019).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Cruz, T., García, L., Álvarez, M. A. & Manzanero, A. L. Sleep quality and memory function in healthy ageing. Neurología (English Edition) 37, 31–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nrleng.2018.10.024 (2022).

Article CAS Google Scholar

Xie, W., Berry, A., Lustig, C., Deldin, P. & Zhang, W. Poor sleep quality and compromised visual working memory capacity. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 25, 583–594. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719000183 (2019).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Hokett, E., Arunmozhi, A., Campbell, J., Verhaeghen, P. & Duarte, A. A systematic review and meta-analysis of individual differences in naturalistic sleep quality and episodic memory performance in young and older adults. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 127, 675–688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.05.010 (2021).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Chakravarty, K. et al. Effect of sleep quality on memory, executive function, and language performance in patients with refractory focal epilepsy and controlled epilepsy versus healthy controls–a prospective study. Epilepsy Behav. 92, 176–183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2018.12.028 (2019).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

van der Heijden, K. B. et al. Chronic sleep reduction is associated with academic achievement and study concentration in higher education students. J. Sleep Res. 27, 165–174. https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12596 (2018).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Brown, T. M. et al. Recommendations for daytime, evening, and nighttime indoor light exposure to best support physiology, sleep, and wakefulness in healthy adults. PLOS Biol. 20, e3001571. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.3001571 (2022).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Siraji, M. et al. An inventory of human light exposure related behaviour. (2023).

Hair, J. F., Risher, J. J., Sarstedt, M. & Ringle, C. M. When to use and how to report the results of PLS-SEM. Eur. Bus. Rev. 31, 2–24. https://doi.org/10.1108/EBR-11-2018-0203 (2019).

Article Google Scholar

Hair, J., Hollingsworth, C. L., Randolph, A. B. & Chong, A. Y. L. An updated and expanded assessment of PLS-SEM in information systems research. Industrial Manag. Data Syst. 117, 442–458. https://doi.org/10.1108/IMDS-04-2016-0130 (2017).

Article Google Scholar

Bollen, K. A. Total, direct, and indirect effects in structural equation models. Sociol. Methodol. 17, 37–69. https://doi.org/10.2307/271028 (1987).

Article Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G. & Buchner, A. G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191 (2007).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Cohen, J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed. edn, (Hillsdale, NJ : L. Erlbaum Associates, 1988).

Hair, J. F., Hult, G. T. M., Ringle, C. M. & Sarstedt, M. A primer on partial least squares structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) 2nd edn. (SAGE Publications Inc, 2017).

MATH Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A. & Tellegen, A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 1063–1070. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 (1988).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R. & Kupfer, D. J. The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28, 193–213. https://doi.org/10.1016/01651781(89)90047-4 (1989).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Manzar, M. D. et al. Dimensionality of the pittsburgh sleep quality index: A systematic review. Health Qual Life Outcomes 16, 89–89. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12955-018-0915-x (2018).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Dunleavy, G. et al. Examining the factor structure of the pittsburgh sleep quality index in a multi-ethnic working population in Singapore. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16, 4590. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph16234590 (2019).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Horne, J. A. & Östberg, O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int. J. Chronobiol. (1976).

Caci, H., Deschaux, O., Adan, A. & Natale, V. Comparing three morningness scales: Age and gender effects, structure and cut-off criteria. Sleep Med. 10, 240–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2008.01.007 (2008).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Team, R. C. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. (2022).

Mateus, S. & Leon, T. d. B. esemComp: ESEM-within-CFA syntax composer. (2022).

Hair, J. F. Partial Least Squares Structural Equation Modeling (PLS-SEM) Using R: A Workbook (Springer International Publishing AG, Cham, 2021).

Book Google Scholar

Siraji, M. Tabledown: Create Publication Quality Tables and Plots. (2022).

Asparouhov, T. & Muthén, B. Exploratory structural equation modeling. Struct. Equ. Model. 16, 397–438. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510903008204 (2009).

Article MathSciNet Google Scholar

Hu, L. T. & Bentler, P. M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model.: A Multidiscip. J. 6, 1–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118 (1999).

Article Google Scholar

MacCallum, R. C., Roznowski, M., Mar, C. M. & Reith, J. V. Alternative strategies for cross-validation of covariance structure models. Multivar. Behav. Res. 29, 1–32 (1994).

Article CAS Google Scholar

MacKenzie, S. B., Podsakoff, P. M. & Jarvis, C. B. The problem of measurement model misspecification in behavioral and organizational research and some recommended solutions. J. Appl. Psychol. 90, 710 (2005).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Hinton, P., McMurray, I. & Brownlow, C. SPSS explained. (2014).

Fornell, C. & Larcker, D. F. Evaluating structural equation models with unobservable variables and measurement error. J. Mark. Res. 18, 39–50 (1981).

Article Google Scholar

Henseler, J., Ringle, C. M. & Sarstedt, M. A new criterion for assessing discriminant validity in variance-based structural equation modeling. J. Acad. Mark. Sci. 43, 115–135 (2015).

Article Google Scholar

Falk, R. F. & Miller, N. B. A primer for soft modeling. (1992).

Drolet, A. L. & Morrison, D. G. Do we really need multiple-item measures in service research?. J. Serv. Res.: JSR 3, 196–204. https://doi.org/10.1177/109467050133001 (2001).

Article Google Scholar

Youngblut, J. M. & Casper, G. R. Focus on psychometrics single-item indicators in nursing research. Res. Nurs. Health 16, 459–465. https://doi.org/10.1002/nur.4770160610 (1993).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Boer, A. G. E. M. D. et al. Is a single-item visual analogue scale as valid, reliable and responsive as multi-item scales in measuring quality of life?. Qual. Life Res. 13, 311–320 (2004).

Article MathSciNet PubMed Google Scholar

Shamir, B. & Kark, R. A single-item graphic scale for the measurement of organizational identification. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 77, 115–123. https://doi.org/10.1348/096317904322915946 (2004).

Article Google Scholar

Fuchs, C. & Diamantopoulos, A. Using single-item measures for construct measurement in management research: Conceptual issues and application guidelines. Die Betriebswirtschaft. 69, 195 (2009).

Google Scholar

Wahl, S., Engelhardt, M., Schaupp, P., Lappe, C. & Ivanov, I. V. The inner clock—Blue light sets the human rhythm. J. Biophoton. 12, 19102. https://doi.org/10.1002/jbio.201900102 (2019).

Article CAS Google Scholar

Münch, M. & Bromundt, V. Light and chronobiology: Implications for health and disease. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 14, 448–453. https://doi.org/10.3188/DCNS.2012.14.4/mmuench (2012).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Figueiro, M. G. et al. Tailored lighting intervention improves measures of sleep, depression, and agitation in persons with Alzheimer’s disease and related dementia living in long-term care facilities. Clin. Interv. Aging 9, 1527–1537. https://doi.org/10.2147/CIA.S68557 (2014).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Figueiro, M. G. & Rea, M. S. Lack of short-wavelength light during the school day delays dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) in middle school students. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 31, 92–96 (2010).

PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

An, M., Colarelli, S. M., O’Brien, K. & Boyajian, M. E. Why we need more nature at work: Effects of natural elements and sunlight on employee mental health and work attitudes. PLoS ONE 11, 155614. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0155614 (2016).

Article CAS Google Scholar

Burns, A. C. et al. Time spent in outdoor light is associated with mood, sleep, and circadian rhythm-related outcomes: A cross-sectional and longitudinal study in over 400,000 UK Biobank participants. J. Affect. Disord. 295, 347–352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2021.08.056 (2021).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Rafique, N. et al. Effects of mobile use on subjective sleep quality. Nat. Sci. Sleep 12, 357–364. https://doi.org/10.2147/nss.S253375 (2020).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Exelmans, L. & Van den Bulck, J. Bedtime mobile phone use and sleep in adults. Soc. Sci. Med. 148, 93–101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2015.11.037 (2016).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Vernon, L., Modecki, K. L. & Barber, B. L. Mobile phones in the bedroom: Trajectories of sleep habits and subsequent adolescent psychosocial development. Child Dev. 89, 66–77. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12836 (2018).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Tosini, G., Ferguson, I. & Tsubota, K. Effects of blue light on the circadian system and eye physiology. Mol. Vis. 22, 61–72 (2016).

CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Schmid, S. R. et al. How smart is it to go to bed with the phone? The Impact of short-wavelength light and affective states on sleep and circadian rhythms. Clocks Sleep 3, 558–580 (2021).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Knufinke, M., Fittkau-Koch, L., Møst, E. I. S., Kompier, M. A. J. & Nieuwenhuys, A. Restricting short-wavelength light in the evening to improve sleep in recreational athletes–a pilot study. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 19, 728–735. https://doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2018.1544278 (2019).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Shechter, A., Quispe, K. A., Mizhquiri Barbecho, J. S., Slater, C. & Falzon, L. Interventions to reduce short-wavelength (“blue”) light exposure at night and their effects on sleep: A systematic review and meta-analysis. SLEEP Adv. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleepadvances/zpaa002 (2020).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Chang, A.-M., Aeschbach, D., Duffy, J. F. & Czeisler, C. A. Evening use of light-emitting eReaders negatively affects sleep, circadian timing, and next-morning alertness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 112, 1232–1237. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1418490112 (2015).

Article ADS CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Chang, A.-M., Scheer, F. A. J. L. & Czeisler, C. A. The human circadian system adapts to prior photic history: The human circadian system adapts to prior light history. J. Physiol. 589, 1095–1102. https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.2010.201194 (2011).

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Carrier, J. & Dumont, M. Sleep propensity and sleep architecture after bright light exposure at three different times of day. J. Sleep Res. 4, 202–211. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.1995.tb00171.x (1995).

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar

Ancoli-Israel, S. et al. Increased light exposure consolidates sleep and strengthens circadian rhythms in severe Alzheimer’s disease patients. Behav. Sleep Med. 1, 22–36. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15402010BSM0101_4 (2003).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Mills, P. R., Tomkins, S. C. & Schlangen, L. J. The effect of high correlated colour temperature office lighting on employee wellbeing and work performance. J. Circadian Rhythms 5, 2. https://doi.org/10.1186/1740-3391-5-2 (2007).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

RautkylÄ, E., Puolakka, M., Tetri, E. & Halonen, L. Effects of correlated colour temperature and timing of light exposure on daytime alertness in lecture environments. J. Light Visual Environ. 34, 59–68. https://doi.org/10.2150/jlve.34.59 (2010).

Article ADS Google Scholar

Boubekri, M., Hull, R. B. & Boyer, L. L. Impact of window size and sunlight penetration on office workers’ mood and satisfaction: A novel way of assessing sunlight. Environ. Behav. 23, 474–493. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013916591234004 (1991).

Article Google Scholar

Houser, K. W. & Esposito, T. Human-centric lighting: Foundational considerations and a five-step design process. Front Neurol 12, 630553. https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2021.630553 (2021).

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar

Taylor, B. J. & Hasler, B. P. Chronotype and mental health: Recent advances. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 20, 59–59. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-018-0925-8 (2018).

Article PubMed Google Scholar

Sakine, A. & Pınar Guzel, O. Effects of chronotype and social jet-lag on neurocognitive functioning. Psikiyatride Güncel Yaklaşimlar 15, 407–417. https://doi.org/10.18863/pgy.1140109 (2023).

Article Google Scholar

Download references

Department of Psychology, Jeffrey Cheah School of Medicine and Health Sciences and Intelligent Lighting Laboratory, Monash University Malaysia, Jalan Lagoon Selatan, 47500, Bandar Sunway, Selangor Darul Ehsan, Malaysia

Mushfiqul Anwar Siraji & Shamsul Haque

Max Planck Institute for Biological Cybernetics, Translational Sensory & Circadian Neuroscience, Tübingen, Germany

Manuel Spitschan

Department of Sport and Health Sciences (TUM SG), Technical University of Munich, Munich, Germany

Manuel Spitschan

Department of Electrical and Computer Systems Engineering and Intelligent Lighting Laboratory, Monash University Malaysia, Jalan Lagoon Selatan, 47500, Bandar Sunway, Selangor Darul Ehsan, Malaysia

Vineetha Kalavally

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

M.A.S., M.S., V.K., and S.H. designed the study and planned for data collection. M.A.S. conducted the formal analysis and prepared all figures and tables. M.A.S., M.S., V.K., and S.H. wrote the main manuscript text. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Correspondence to Shamsul Haque.

The authors declare no competing interests.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and Permissions

Siraji, M.A., Spitschan, M., Kalavally, V. et al. Light exposure behaviors predict mood, memory and sleep quality. Sci Rep 13, 12425 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-39636-y

Download citation

Received: 18 January 2023

Accepted: 28 July 2023

Published: 01 August 2023

DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-39636-y

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

SHARE